16

According to the New York Times:

ONE of the most striking scientific discoveries about religion in recent years is that going to church weekly is good for you. Religious attendance — at least, religiosity — boosts the immune system and decreases blood pressure. It may add as much as two to three years to your life.

Every time I read "boost your immune system", I am highly suspicious and we did debunk a fake study on religion before.

Is this op-ed based on decent studies conducted by unbiased researchers?

Oddthinking
  • 140,378
  • 46
  • 548
  • 638
Sklivvz
  • 78,578
  • 29
  • 321
  • 428
  • Maybe living longer is due to being born earlier (but not dead yet) at a time when people were more likely to be educated to go to church more. – Mike Dunlavey Apr 23 '13 at 13:04
  • 5
    Uh, oh: "My mother is the daughter of a Baptist minister, my father (a doctor) the son of Christian Scientists", from the author's bio. Undisclosed bias. – Sklivvz Apr 23 '13 at 13:11
  • 7
    I seem to recall reading about this in the Economist, but cannot find it now. IIRC, there is a correlation between longevity and religious belief, but the reason was attributed to religious people being significantly more likely to subscribe to life-sustaining treatment in spite of low quality of life or morbidity whereas less religious people were more likely to agree to be taken off treatment once their quality of life degraded. The inquiry went deeper and found that religious folk were more likely to have a stronger fear of death. I'll look for the article - it was fascinating. – Brian M. Hunt Apr 23 '13 at 13:12
  • 6
    I would guess that it's not "going to religious services" that causes the effect, but rather "having a supportive and accessible community of peers" that lies at the heart of this effect. – JasonR Apr 23 '13 at 13:19
  • 3
    More than half a year of your life you are spending in church if you visit every week for 2h over 50 years. – Stephan Schielke Apr 23 '13 at 13:20
  • 5
    I wonder if the studies (if any) account for the base-level of healthiness required to be able travel to church, and the base-level of wealth and leisure of being able to afford a couple of hours free from jobs, chores and sleeping each week. i.e. the very ill and the working poor may have less capacity to attend church in the first place. – Oddthinking Apr 23 '13 at 13:29
  • I doubt unless they do some physical exercises there in the church! – Persian Cat Apr 23 '13 at 15:06
  • For clarifications, when this says "church" are we talking about something along the lines of Catholic mass, or any regularly scheduled religious group activities of any faith? – rjzii Apr 23 '13 at 16:32
  • @RobZ the claim is as advertised. Whatever definition of church that the study mentioned, if found, has used should be part of the answer. – Sklivvz Apr 23 '13 at 17:03
  • Might take a bit of digging to really get at the heart of things then. I don't see a single publication on the [author's CV](http://www.stanford.edu/dept/anthropology/cgi-bin/web/?q=node/105) to start pulling apart, but on the same token she does appear to have at least studied the topic quite a bit. – rjzii Apr 23 '13 at 17:38
  • 2
    I think I've heard claims that going to bingo also lengthens your life span. So it's not necessarily something special about church. – Andrew Grimm Apr 24 '13 at 04:43
  • 2
    Let us assume for one minute you're assumption is correct. Assume I go to church for 2 hour every week from birth to age 80. Hours spent in church = 2*52*80 = 8320 hours. Assuming you are awake 16 hours a day, 8320/16 = 520 "waking days". This is about 1.5 years. So if church adds 2 years to your life, you're only making half a year of profit. Similarly, exercising every day apparently can also extend your life by a few years, but when you add up all the time it takes to exercise for 1 hour, it equates to 5 years of your waking life (if you live to 80). So if exercising doesn't add 5 years to – Kenshin Apr 24 '13 at 05:20
  • 1
    @Chris you could use your time in church to sleep, that'd make the net benefit greater I assume ;) – jwenting Apr 24 '13 at 05:59
  • @Chris "Hours spent in church". This assumes the time spent in the church is lost. Some religious people may not share this view and may include such time to the "added to life" column in their balance. The same with exercise. (I am keen biker and hiker and I would definitely not count time spent on that as lost, quite the contrary, it is one of my favourite activities). – Suma Apr 24 '13 at 06:36
  • 1
    @Chris - I agree with the others in that you really can't apply a calculation like that and look at just the net gain in time. You also need to take into account if there is an overall improvement in quality of life, if regular attendance of church allows someone to not worry about some things in their life, it may reduce their overall stress which can also improve their lifespan and quality of life. – rjzii Apr 24 '13 at 12:58
  • @RobZ, I agree with what you say, and if people enjoy going to church to spend time with God and other christians, the 2 hours per week isn't wasted life, so my calculation is flawed. My calculation is more correct however if people read the article and go to church just to try to extend their lives when they don't even like going otherwise. – Kenshin Apr 25 '13 at 00:36
  • 1
    I had always a problem with these kinds of claims because no one of claimers note to the fact that every enjoyable matter can help people to get ride of stress,disease and depression. One by sport, one by love, one by art and another one by religion and church. It is not important what the matter is. – Persian Cat Apr 25 '13 at 22:32
  • @Oddthinking That problem plagues a lot of longevity research--a lot of things that supposedly correlate with a long life are by their nature a screen for a reasonable degree of health. If they weren't associated with longevity it would be a surprise. – Loren Pechtel Jul 22 '18 at 00:52

2 Answers2

9

Yes, going to church can influence your health according to scientific studies. However, this doesn't say that the same effect can't be achieved by other means, or that the religious part of church going is the determining factor, and not the social or psychological. In other words, all the scientific articles on the subject are merely describing phenomenons and don't analyze them, but saying that more research is needed, which means that the real reasons and factors that affect the health of church goers are yet unknown.

The author of the article seems to suggest this as well, but less willingly:

Social support is no doubt part of the story. At the evangelical churches I’ve studied as an anthropologist, people really did seem to look out for one another. They showed up with dinner when friends were sick and sat to talk with them when they were unhappy. The help was sometimes surprisingly concrete. Perhaps a third of the church members belonged to small groups that met weekly to talk about the Bible and their lives ... A study conducted in North Carolina found that frequent churchgoers had larger social networks, with more contact with, more affection for, and more kinds of social support from those people than their unchurched counterparts. And we know that social support is directly tied to better health.

Healthy behavior is no doubt another part. Certainly many churchgoers struggle with behaviors they would like to change, but on average, regular church attendees drink less, smoke less, use fewer recreational drugs and are less sexually promiscuous than others.

And at the end of the article she calls it placebo and suggests that studying this phenomena would help up use this effect to better people's life.

Eventually, this may teach us how to harness the “placebo” effect ... We do not understand the placebo effect, but we know it is real. That is, we have increasingly better evidence that what anthropologists would call “symbolic healing” has real physical effects on the body. At the heart of some of these mysterious effects may be the capacity to trust that what can only be imagined may be real, and be good.

Studies:

Sadly, the author doesn't provide the references to all the studies she cites in the article, which makes our job harder, and her arguments less persuasive. And she is a professor in Stanford, she really should know better that this.

The main article on which it's all based is this meta-study by Daniel E. Hall, MD. However the study itself is not too enthusiastic as to the results:

Background: A recent meta-analysis demonstrates a robust but small association between weekly religious attendance and longer life. However, the practical significance of this finding remains controversial.

...

Conclusion: The real-world, practical significance of regular religious attendance is comparable to commonly recommended therapies, and rough estimates even suggest that religious attendance may be more cost-effective than statins. Religious attendance is not a mode of medical therapy, but these findings warrant more and better quality research designed to examine the associations between religion and health, and the potential relevance such associations might have for medical practice.

The author doesn't suggest that churchgoing is the cure, but that there is something in it that should be studied further.

In the July issue of the same magazine a letter was published by Peter S. Millard, MD, PhD, which says that the study wasn't really good because it disregarded all the other factors that sarround the issue, I really liked it, so I'm citing it fully here:

Dr. Hall bases his analysis on observational data that are of questionable validity. The fact that churchgoers live longer than people who do not attend church may very well have nothing to do with churchgoing but may result from uncontrolled confounding.

Observational studies that showed a benefit of exogenous estrogens in postmenopausal women were debunked by the Women’s Health Initiative Randomized Controlled Trial.1 The accepted explanation for the discrepancy between the observational findings and the experimental results is that women who took estrogens were systematically different from non-users in ways which resulted in improved outcomes (eg, reduced coronary disease).

Barrett-Connor referred to this as the “healthy user effect.”2 The inability to control for the healthy user effect resulted in the biased findings of many observational studies of estrogen use.

Similarly, churchgoers are systematically different from non-churchgoers in ways that are difficult to measure but are likely to result in improved health outcomes that may have nothing to do with churchgoing. Churchgoers are more likely to be employed, have intact families, and are less likely to be homebound by illness or disability. Until the healthy attender effect can be controlled for, it is unwise to attempt to make any inferences about the effect that churchgoing has on health.

The study that claims that churchgoers have a greater social network is probably this one:

Frequent churchgoers report larger social networks, more contact with network members, more types of social support received, and more favorable perception of the quality of their social relationships than do their unchurched counterparts. Farther, most of these empirical patterns withstand statistical controls for a wide range of covariates.

This study suggest that church going can relieve stress, but the more interesting part is that it depends on the church and denomination you go to:

In this study we examined the stress-buffering effect of an intrinsic religious orientation for a community sample of adult Protestant churchgoers. At time 1, participants completed measures of intrinsic religiousness, religious activity, and dysphoria. At time 2, 8 months later, they completed measures of negative life events and dysphoria. For participants from liberal Protestant churches (e.g., Methodist), intrinsic religiousness served as a stress-buffer in the prediction of time 1–time 2 residual change in dysphoria. This effect was nonsignificant for participants from more conservative Protestant churches (e.g., Baptist). Similarly, single-item measures of religion's importance, frequency of prayer, and frequency of church attendance served as stress-buffers for liberal participants, but not for conservative participants. The nonsignificant effects for the latter participants are attributable to their restricted range on the religion variables. The results for the former participants suggest that religious “commitment” is an individual difference variable that influences adjustment to negative life events

Regarding the immune system, there is this study that suggest that male gay HIV-positive churchgoers have a higher T-cell count, which shows that church going affects positively on the immune system

This study examines the relationship between religiosity and the affective and immune status of 106 HIV-seropositive mildly symptomatic gay men (CDC stage B). All men completed an intake interview, a set of psychosocial questionnaires, and provided a venous blood sample. Factor analysis of 12 religiously oriented response items revealed two distinct aspects to religiosity: religious coping and religious behavior. Religious coping (e.g., placing trust in God, seeking comfort in religion) was significantly associated with lower scores on the Beck Depression Inventory, but not with specific immune markers. On the other hand, religious behavior (e.g., service attendance, prayer, spiritual discussion, reading religious literature) was significantly associated with higher T-helper-inducer cell (CD4+) counts and higher CD4+ percentages, but not with depression. Regression analyses indicated that religiosity’s associations with affective and immune status was not mediated by the subjects’ sense of self-efficacy or ability to actively cope with their health situation. The associations between religiosity and affective and immune status also appear to be independent of symptom status. Self-efficacy, however, did appear to contribute uniquely and significantly to lower depression scores. Our results show that an examination considering both subject religiosity as well as sense of self-efficacy may predict depressive symptoms in HIV-infected gay men better than an examination that considers either variable in isolation.

This study however has a very small sample size (try to find a lot of HIV positive gay men that go to church), and should be taken with caution.

This study shows that churchgoing doesn't affect blood pressure, and hypertension, but that prayer increases hypertension, and spirituality increases diastolic blood pressure. This study may contradict the article in some aspects, but it shows that religiosity can affect a persons physical status and health, which (in my opinion) is the main point of the article.

Researchers have established the role of heredity and lifestyle in the occurrence of hypertension, but the potential role of psychosocial factors, especially religiosity, is less understood. This paper analyzes the relationship between multiple dimensions of religiosity and systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, and hypertension using data taken from the Chicago Community Adult Health Study, a probability sample of adults (N = 3105) aged 18 and over living in the city of Chicago, USA. Of the primary religiosity variables examined here, attendance and public participation were not significantly related to the outcomes. Prayer was associated with an increased likelihood of hypertension, and spirituality was associated with increased diastolic blood pressure. The addition of several other religiosity variables to the models did not appear to affect these findings. However, variables for meaning and forgiveness were associated with lower diastolic blood pressure and a decreased likelihood of hypertension outcomes. These findings emphasize the importance of analyzing religiosity as a multidimensional phenomenon. This study should be regarded as a first step toward systematically analyzing a complex relationship.

And there are some some studies, all show that there can be and is correlation between churchgoing and religiosity and a person's health:

Is going to church good or bad for you? Denomination, attendance and mental health of children in West Scotland

Is Church Attendance Associated With Latinas' Health Practices and Self-reported Health?

In conclusion:

Like every aspect of human life, churchgoing and religion may have affect on our health, but the mechanism behind it, the reasons and what aspects of our health are affected are still unknown, and require much more high quality research. All the study that available today, don't try to explain the phenomenon only to describe it.

A serious study, which will be able to make the right differentiation of the elements that are unique to religion and churchgoing, is still needed, and until it's done, we can't say what is the aspect of this particular activity that has affect on us.

SIMEL
  • 29,037
  • 14
  • 123
  • 139
  • with regards to the authors references in the article, with all fairness it is a newspaper Op-Ed so we can't hold it to the same standards for sources as a journal article. – rjzii Apr 24 '13 at 15:30
  • @RobZ, You are right off-course, but in this case we have a prof. talking about a specific source, while she doesn't have to put the source, it's a good practice, and while not mandatory, it should be at least encouraged. – SIMEL Apr 24 '13 at 15:53
  • You should edit the first sentence of your answer to be more accurate. There is no evidence that going to a church has the effects described in the question, merely that people who attend a church on a regular basis are often a little healthier. That's hardly surprising considering that people with less income (which is known to be a direct cause of poorer heath) have less opportunity to spend free time socialising. –  Nov 02 '15 at 12:54
4

Maybe, but church attendance per se may not be the cause.

This is an extremely complicated topic that since there are many variables involved and as the as the author even noted in the article,

Eventually, this may teach us how to harness the “placebo” effect — a terrible word, because it suggests an absence of intervention rather than the presence of a healing mechanism that depends neither on pharmaceuticals nor on surgery. We do not understand the placebo effect, but we know it is real. That is, we have increasingly better evidence that what anthropologists would call “symbolic healing” has real physical effects on the body. At the heart of some of these mysterious effects may be the capacity to trust that what can only be imagined may be real, and be good.

Finding a direct correlation between religiosity and life expectancy can be difficult largely due to the number of variables involved. For example, it is sometimes speculated that religious people are less likely to fear death; however, studies of cancer patients have shown that while there is a connection between the strength of a person's conventions and their anxiety towards dying,

He found that patients with strong religious faith, and those with none, were least distressed about dying.

The same study when on to find that religiosity had no statistical impact upon the patient outcomes or life expectancy. So while strong convictions may help your mental state, it doesn't appear to do much to help your life expectancy when you have an illness such as cancer.

Coming at things differently, the author cites a number of anecdotes about regular church attendee and the social support networks that grow up around them. This has been studied and it has been shown that strong social support carries over into subjective life expectancy and overall quality of life. Having adequate emotional support through stressful events has also been shown to been tied to ones ability to survive those events as well.

In this same vein of thought the articles author notes the following anecdote,

One evening, a young woman in a group I joined began to cry. Her dentist had told her that she needed a $1,500 procedure, and she didn’t have the money. To my amazement, our small group — most of them students — simply covered the cost, by anonymous donation.

This leads to the possibility that the social networks involved with churches might also assist members through stressful periods in their life. Stress and lower life expectancies have been linked and even modest amounts of stress relief from the social network, or even just through the weekly ritual of attending church services1.

However, it is also important to note that literature reviews have found that studies involving religion and health effects can be methodologically unsound,

Of the 266 articles published in the year 2000 and identified by the Medline search, only 17% were relevant to claims of health benefits associated with religious involvement. About half of the articles cited in the comprehensive reviews were irrelevant to these claims. Of those that actually were relevant, many either had significant methodological flaws or were misrepresented, leaving only a few articles that could truly be described as demonstrating beneficial effects of religious involvement. We conclude that there is little empirical basis for assertions that religious involvement or activity is associated with beneficial health outcomes.

However, in someways this brings back to the authors comments on the placebo effect and the difficulty in determining exactly why a correlation between church attendance and life expectancy may exist. Correlation does not imply causation it it may not be the church attendance but the positive attitude of a group of attendees could be affecting the overall life expectancy.

In short, while there do appear to be links between church attendance and overall life expectancy, we don't know why this link exists and it may not be possible to isolate church attendance in and of itself enough to determine the answer.


  1. This is some speculation on my part since I didn't have time to search for studies; however, just the regular ritual of church services may allow people to "decompress" and relieve some of their stress which could improve their overall health.
rjzii
  • 16,884
  • 4
  • 92
  • 102
  • "We conclude that there is little empirical basis for assertions that religious involvement or activity is associated with beneficial health outcomes." That appears to be the key line in this answer. I am surprised it isn't given more prominence over the softer statements. – Oddthinking Apr 24 '13 at 14:28
  • @Oddthinking - To be honest I wasn't really sure how to line everything up for the response. I could easily sit down and write a couple more pages on the topic after all of the reading but most if it boiled down to "too many variables", "churches expose attendees to other positive influences", and "most of the studies aren't that good." – rjzii Apr 24 '13 at 14:30